Paso 1 (hecho): grabar gente silabificando.
Paso 2: Poner las grabaciones en línea para que otros raters den su juicio (i.e., ¿esta persona ha dicho la ka pi aisto? - nativos (de donde?) - cuantos participantes? - cuantas opciones? (mirar doc a ver si hay tendencias) - que se nos olvida?
Paso 3: Reclutar más participantes para que hagan la tarea de silabificación en línea y las opciones serían las respuestas más comunes del piloto (paso 1) que han sido confirmadas por raters (paso 2). - ven texto - escogen entre dos opciones - nativos (de donde?) (dos grupos?) - cuantos participantes?
The following plots show results from the syllabification task. Four syllable nonse words containing a CGVGC sequence in the penultimate syllable were presented on a screen. Participants read the words out loud, syllable by syllable. The plots show the overall distribution of the responses, which have been classified in the following manner:
Figure 1 plots the proportion of responses across all data (collapsing across the pre-vocalic glide and onset). We observe that tripthongs were produced in approximately 45% of the targets. A production containing a hiatus made up roughly 30% of the data, followed by a simplification of some sort (~25% of the time).
If we combine hiatus
and simplification
(Figure 2), we see that they
make up approximately 60% of the data.
Overall, the task provides evidence supporting the hypothesis that pre-vocalic glides can be part of the onset in this variety of Spanish.
To consider:
Statistical analysis:
(1 + glide + consonant | participant)
(1 + glide + consonant | item)
The following plots illustrate the results from the carrier sentence task. These analyses test the hypothesis that pre-vocalic glides will be disallowed if preceded by a palatal consonant. To test this, we measure duration, F1, and intensity of the pre-vocalic glide in two environments: after a palatal consonant and after any other consonant. If pre-vocalic glides are blocked after palatals, we expect to observe differences in overall duration. Figure 3 plots the duration data.
We see a longer duration for glide segments following a palatal consonant (contrary to the hypothesis). There are two possible explanations (that I can think of). 1) The participants are able to produce a glide in this context (at least in some cases) because the preceding consonant is not palatal (i.e., ‘ch’ is surfacing as [ʃ] or the fact that it is alveo-palatal is different enough to allow a glide afterwards). 2) The participants are not producing a glide, but rather are lengthening the onset.
We can examine the plausibility of (1) by looking at the formant trajectory over the course of the segment. If the participants are able to produce a glide in some contexts but not others we should see differences in F1 as a function of the preceding consonant.
Again, the second possibility is that participants are lengthening the palatal consonant (not producing a glide) because of the fact that they cannot produce both. In other words, they might be trying to produce something, but because the target is illicit, they resort to lengthening the onset. If this is indeed what is happening, then we would expect the intensity of the lengthened segment to be lower from that of a glide. Why? Because more consonant-like productions have lower intensity than more vowel-like productions. Thus we analyze the time course of intensity in order to see how the two environments differ as they near the following [a].